Longitudinal Changes in Pronoun Reversal in Children with ASD and TD Children

Friday, May 18, 2012
Sheraton Hall (Sheraton Centre Toronto)
2:00 PM
M. Cheng1, N. Khetrapal2, K. Demuth2, D. A. Fein1 and L. Naigles1, (1)University of Connecticut, Storrs, CT, (2)Macquarie University, Sydney, Australia
Background: Pronoun reversal (e.g., using “you” for “I”) is a striking characteristic of ASD language (Fay, 1979). However, the actual incidence of reversals is still unknown. Previous research has reported 15% reversals in 5-7-year-old children with ASD (Tager-Flusberg, 1994), and around 85% reversals in a two-year-old child with Asperger’s syndrome (Evans & Demuth, in press). It is also unclear what proportion of ASD and TD children reverse.

Objectives: To assess these issues we examined spontaneous pronoun use in children with ASD and TD controls in a longitudinal study of language development. 

Methods: Children (n=10 ASD, 18 TD) were visited every four months for two years. At visit 1, ASD children had a mean age of 33 months, had begun intensive ABA therapy, had mean Mullen Visual Reception T-scores of 45.6, and produced 159.7 words on the CDI-1. TD controls had a mean age of 20 months, mean Mullen VR T-scores of 53, and produced 118.8 words on the CDI-1.

At each visit, children engaged in 30-minute semi-structured play sessions with their parents. The children’s spontaneous speech was coded for the occurrence of 1st and 2nd person pronouns, and for whether each pronoun was correct or reversed. Data from visits 1, 3, and 5 are reported here. 

Results: Pronoun use increased with age for both groups, the ASD group producing an average of 12.8 (SD=14.15) at visit 1, 36.5 (SD=36.12) at visit 3, and 46.55 (SD=37.97), and the TD group producing an average of 3.22 (SD=6.84) at visit 1, 37.61 (SD=32.02) at visit 3, and 55.88 (SD=24.31) at visit 5 (group differences ns). Both groups produced many more pronouns referring to self than other (ranges 63%-77%) at all visits (group differences ns). Percent of pronoun uses that were reversals also varied across visits; the ASD group produced 14.12% reversals at visit 1, 3.14% reversals at visit 3, and 8.56% reversals at visit 5, and the TD group produced 2.5% reversals at visit 1, 2.25% reversals at visit 3, and 1.2% reversals at visit 5. The ASD group produced a marginally higher percentage of pronoun reversals only at visit 5 (t(9) = 2.17, p =.057). Developmental changes were seen in both groups concerning which pronouns were reversed more: Children with ASD used “I” for “you” more at visit 1, but “you” for “I” more at visits 3 and 5 (X2 = 5.47, p = .065); TD children’ used “you” for “I” more at visit 1, but “I” for “you” more at visits 3 and 5 (X2 = 6.83, p = .033). The groups differed in their patterns of reversals at visits 1 (X2 = 5.6, p = .018) and 3 (X2 = 5.61, p = .018).

Conclusions: Both TD and ASD children increased pronoun use with age, and both groups produced pronoun reversals. Although the incidence of reversals was lower than previously reported, the ASD children produced a higher proportion of pronouns as reversed, and more frequently used “you” to refer to themselves. Pronoun reversals may be both contextually- and developmentally-sensitive.

| More